Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Ahead of Print
    • Past Issues
    • Supplements
    • Article Type
  • Specialty
    • Articles by Specialty
  • CME/MOC
    • Articles
    • Calendar
  • Info For
    • Manuscript Submission
    • Authors & Reviewers
    • Subscriptions
    • About CCJM
    • Contact Us
    • Media Kit
  • Conversations with Leaders
  • Conference Coverage
    • Kidney Week 2024
    • CHEST 2024
    • ACR Convergence 2023
    • Kidney Week 2023
    • ObesityWeek 2023
    • IDWeek 2023
    • CHEST 2023
    • MDS 2023
    • IAS 2023
    • ACP 2023
    • AAN 2023
    • ACC / WCC 2023
    • AAAAI Meeting 2023
    • ACR Convergence 2022
    • Kidney Week 2022
    • AIDS 2022
  • Other Publications
    • www.clevelandclinic.org

User menu

  • Register
  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
Cleveland Clinic Journal of Medicine
  • Other Publications
    • www.clevelandclinic.org
  • Register
  • Log in
Cleveland Clinic Journal of Medicine

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Content
    • Current Issue
    • Ahead of Print
    • Past Issues
    • Supplements
    • Article Type
  • Specialty
    • Articles by Specialty
  • CME/MOC
    • Articles
    • Calendar
  • Info For
    • Manuscript Submission
    • Authors & Reviewers
    • Subscriptions
    • About CCJM
    • Contact Us
    • Media Kit
  • Conversations with Leaders
  • Conference Coverage
    • Kidney Week 2024
    • CHEST 2024
    • ACR Convergence 2023
    • Kidney Week 2023
    • ObesityWeek 2023
    • IDWeek 2023
    • CHEST 2023
    • MDS 2023
    • IAS 2023
    • ACP 2023
    • AAN 2023
    • ACC / WCC 2023
    • AAAAI Meeting 2023
    • ACR Convergence 2022
    • Kidney Week 2022
    • AIDS 2022
Commentary

COVID-19 diagnostic testing and the psychology of precautions fatigue

Gregory W. Ruhnke, MD, MS, MPH
Cleveland Clinic Journal of Medicine January 2021, 88 (1) 19-21; DOI: https://doi.org/10.3949/ccjm.88a.20086
Gregory W. Ruhnke
Assistant Professor, Section of Hospital Medicine, Department of Medicine, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: [email protected]
  • Article
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

A 65-year-old man is hospitalized with mild dyspnea and myalgias. After an initial nasopharyngeal swab is negative for SARS-CoV-2, the hospital staff requests that special respiratory precautions be discontinued without a second test. They cite a low observed yield of repeated testing and the burden of precautions on workflow.

Hospital policies regarding the necessity of a confirmatory second negative test are based on the chances of an initial false negative in a population with an estimated probability of disease. However, staff perception of residual disease risk is based on the chances of a patient’s having COVID-19 despite having tested negative. Psychological fatigue arises from incessant gowning, gloving, and masking among patients extremely unlikely to have COVID-19. This fatigue leads to a reduced vigilance that degrades the value of precautions across all patients in isolation.

PRECAUTIONS FATIGUE IS ANALOGOUS TO ALARM FATIGUE

The COVID-19 crisis is an opportunity to reframe clinical decision-making. Despite decades of teaching clinical reasoning, promulgating practice guidelines, and advocating shared decision-making to allocate scarce resources, excessive testing and monitoring persist. One consequence is alarm fatigue,1 caused by false-positive (clinically insignificant) notifications of status changes—when alarms persistently sound with little purpose, people stop paying attention.

A generation ago, analogous concerns generated surgeons’ desire for human immunodeficiency virus screening to enable selective implementation of special precautions rather than universally applied intensive measures. The large number of COVID-19 patients and hospitalized “patients under investigation” (PUIs) for whom special respiratory precautions are ordered is creating a previously unseen degree of “precautions fatigue.” The current practice environment represents an opportunity for experiential cognizance of well-intentioned efforts at error avoidance that become diluted through overly broad application.

With the increased incidence of disease in the community, more readily available testing, recognition of nosocomial transmission, and fewer admissions for diseases other than COVID-19, the proportion of hospitalized patients being evaluated for COVID-19 is markedly higher than it was earlier in the pandemic. When testing was scarce, costly, and time-consuming, patients were tested only if they had multiple COVID-19 symptoms and a known exposure or relevant travel history. With increasing incidence, populations being tested have broadened, escalating the number of patients considered at risk and creating anxiety among healthcare workers through both the presence of true disease and the ubiquitous respiratory precautions signs on isolation rooms.2

CONSEQUENCES OF PRECAUTIONS FATIGUE

Unless incremental increases in testing are specifically targeted at high-prevalence groups, higher testing rates generally result in both a lower proportion of positive test results (lower diagnostic yield due to reduced average pretest probability) and less-severe disease, on average, among those who test positive.3 Since hospitalized PUIs affect behavior of hospital staff and deplete personal protective equipment, it is important to understand their effect on the psychology of inpatient healthcare workers and on disease transmission.

Precautions fatigue has become evident in hospitals, reducing the average effectiveness of precautions the same way that alarm fatigue desensitizes staff, reducing the average clinical benefit of each alarm triggered.1 Precautions fatigue results in guideline-discordant reuse of personal protective equipment, reduced attention to facial protection (given barriers to its access), less-vigorous encouragement of patient mask-donning, and dramatic reductions in direct patient contact.

Early in the pandemic, a substantial proportion of hospitalized PUIs were eventually diagnosed with COVID-19, for several reasons. Testing had a relatively high yield because protocols restricted testing to high-risk populations, as understanding of the clinical spectrum of disease was poor and testing capability and availability were limited. Testing had higher sensitivity due to policies (inadvertently) restricting testing to patients with greater viral shedding and substantial nasal secretions.4 Also, there was a time lag between sending the sample and getting the result, so that for PUIs, initial results of nasopharyngeal swabs were not available for 2 to 3 days, leaving them at a higher instantaneous probability of disease, ie, probably at one point in time.

More recently, when the clinical presentation suggests the disease, most hospitalized PUIs have received negative results on their first test before being admitted, which reduces the probability of disease to the false-negative rate. The exceptions are those with a positive first test (who would be admitted to a COVID-19 unit and would no longer be considered under investigation) and those with a pretest probability sufficiently low for liberation from PUI status after a single negative test.

FALSE-NEGATIVE RATES VS FALSE-OMISSION RATES

Diagnostic tests have known or imperfectly estimated sensitivities and specificities, which depend on the tests themselves and factors such as sample quality, body habitus, and timing of sampling relative to the natural history of disease. Although the sensitivity of polymerase chain reaction testing of a nasopharyngeal swab for SARS-CoV-2 has not been established, many hospitals have been using an estimate of 70%, based on early studies.5

As a test characteristic, sensitivity does not generally depend on disease prevalence in the population tested. However, if less-symptomatic populations and asymptomatic preprocedure inpatients are tested, lower levels of viral shedding and lower volume of nasal secretions among those tested may decrease the sensitivity, as studies of influenza have demonstrated.4 Therefore, while we assume the sensitivity is 70%, a lower value would increase both the false-negative rate and the false-omission rate (ie, 1 minus the negative predictive value).

A relatively low positive predictive value and high false-discovery rate (1 minus the positive predictive value) have been implicated in the poor response times associated with alarm fatigue.1 However, reductions over time in attentiveness to precautions among staff caring for COVID-19 PUIs is due to the aggregate of both true and perceived lower prevalence of disease, considering all those tested as the denominator. The true prevalence of disease is decreased due to increased testing rates and the fact that most PUIs have had a negative test result. The latter leads to a probability of disease given as: P (negative test | COVID +) ie, the probability that a diseased patient would test negative, which is equal to the false-negative rate times the true prevalence within the tested population. However, staff perceptions of residual disease prevalence will be based on the false-omission rate, given as:

P (COVID + | negative test) ie, the probability that a patient who tests negative has COVID, rather than the false-negative rate.

Two relevant considerations arise from the distinction between precautions policies among those with a first negative test, which are often based on the false-negative rate. First, the false-omission rate varies much more with prevalence than the false-negative rate. For example, assuming the sensitivity is 70% and the prevalence is 10%, the false-negative rate and false-omission rate are nearly identical at 0.03 and 0.0326, respectively. However, if the prevalence is 50%, the false-negative rate and false-omission rate diverge to 0.15 and 0.233, respectively, creating clinically significant relative and absolute differences.

Moreover, let us assume the specificity is 99%. Based on Fagan’s nomogram and a calculated negative likelihood ratio of 0.30, the post-test probability of disease at prevalence rates of 10% and 50% would be 0.03 and 0.23, nearly indistinguishable from the false-omission rates.

Second, precautions-laden inpatient units reduce the perceived COVID-19 prevalence among isolation precautions hospital rooms, caused by true diminishing marginal returns to incremental increases in testing that occur as a result of surveillance bias. There is a substantial literature on the psychology of false positives,6 but little empirical evidence regarding cognitive processes surrounding false negatives. Despite this, previous literature shows that everyday human cognitive judgments follow the statistical principles of perception and reveal a close correspondence between implicit human probabilistic models and empiric statistical models.7 Cognitive heuristics are operationalized based on predictive values and their complements, such that, in the same way that false-discovery rates drive alarm fatigue, false-omission rates are likely the principal psychological driver of precautions fatigue. The commonality of empiric statistics and psychologic processes is a rationale for using decrements in the false-omission rate as a quantitative measure of the contribution of incremental testing to diagnostic yield.

RETURN TO THE CASE SCENARIO

Since false-omission rates are more dependent on prevalence than false-negative rates, they should be considered in COVID-19 liberation pathways. Practically, this means that, in addition to the well-recognized adverse effects of precautions,8 decisions about liberating patients from COVID-19 precautions should consider the reduced clinical benefit of isolation if precautions fatigue causes noncompliance as an unintended consequence. Medical literature suggests that contact precautions are used more often than evidence warrants, which creates more harm than benefit.8 A learning healthcare system should use the COVID-19 experience to incorporate the real impact of precautions fatigue into decision algorithms and clinical pathways.

Protection of hospital staff and limiting nosocomial transmission of COVID-19 are critical. However, overuse of special precautions in patients unlikely to have true disease can lead to precautions fatigue that diminishes vigilance among staff, thereby vitiating the value of isolation. Hospital leadership should assertively select patients for testing and retesting, considering that precautions fatigue markedly reduces the average effectiveness of precautions for all isolation rooms.

DISCLOSURES

The author reports no relevant financial relationships which, in the context of his contribution, could be perceived as a potential conflict of interest.

  • Copyright © 2021 The Cleveland Clinic Foundation. All Rights Reserved.

REFERENCES

  1. ↵
    1. Winters BD,
    2. Cvach MM,
    3. Bonafide CP,
    4. et al
    . Technological distractions (part 2): a summary of approaches to manage clinical alarms with intent to reduce alarm fatigue. Crit Care Med 2018; 46(1):130–137. doi:10.1097/CCM.0000000000002803
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  2. ↵
    1. Shanafelt T,
    2. Ripp J,
    3. Trockel M
    . Understanding and addressing sources of anxiety among health care professionals during the COVID-19 pandemic. JAMA 2020; 323(21):2133–2134. doi:10.1001/jama.2020.5893
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Verity R,
    2. Okell LC,
    3. Dorigatti I,
    4. et al
    . Estimates of the severity of coronavirus disease 2019: a model-based analysis. Lancet Infect Dis 2020; 20(6):669–677. doi:10.1016/S1473-3099(20)30243-7.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Chartrand C,
    2. Leeflang MM,
    3. Minion J,
    4. Brewer T,
    5. Pai M
    . Accuracy of rapid influenza diagnostic tests: a meta-analysis. Ann Intern Med 2012; 156(7):500–511. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-156-7-201204030-00403
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Zitek T
    . The appropriate use of testing for COVID-19. West J Emerg Med 2020; 21(3):470–472. doi:10.5811/westjem.2020.4.47370
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  6. ↵
    1. Simmons JP,
    2. Nelson LD,
    3. Simonsohn U
    . False-positive psychology: undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychol Sci 2011; 22(11):1359–1366. doi:10.1177/0956797611417632
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Griffiths TL,
    2. Tenenbaum JB
    . Optimal predictions in everyday cognition. Psychol Sci 2006; 17(9):767–773. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01780.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Young K,
    2. Doernberg SB,
    3. Snedecor RF,
    4. Mallin E
    . Things we do for no reason: contact precautions for MRSA and VRE. J Hosp Med 2019; 14(3):178–180. doi:10.12788/jhm.3126
    OpenUrlCrossRef
PreviousNext
Back to top

In this issue

Cleveland Clinic Journal of Medicine: 88 (1)
Cleveland Clinic Journal of Medicine
Vol. 88, Issue 1
1 Jan 2021
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • Index by author
  • Complete Issue (PDF)
Print
Download PDF
Article Alerts
Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on Cleveland Clinic Journal of Medicine.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
COVID-19 diagnostic testing and the psychology of precautions fatigue
(Your Name) has sent you a message from Cleveland Clinic Journal of Medicine
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the Cleveland Clinic Journal of Medicine web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
COVID-19 diagnostic testing and the psychology of precautions fatigue
Gregory W. Ruhnke
Cleveland Clinic Journal of Medicine Jan 2021, 88 (1) 19-21; DOI: 10.3949/ccjm.88a.20086

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Share
COVID-19 diagnostic testing and the psychology of precautions fatigue
Gregory W. Ruhnke
Cleveland Clinic Journal of Medicine Jan 2021, 88 (1) 19-21; DOI: 10.3949/ccjm.88a.20086
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Linkedin Share Button

Jump to section

  • Article
    • PRECAUTIONS FATIGUE IS ANALOGOUS TO ALARM FATIGUE
    • CONSEQUENCES OF PRECAUTIONS FATIGUE
    • FALSE-NEGATIVE RATES VS FALSE-OMISSION RATES
    • RETURN TO THE CASE SCENARIO
    • DISCLOSURES
    • REFERENCES
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

Related Articles

  • No related articles found.
  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited By...

  • No citing articles found.
  • Google Scholar

More in this TOC Section

  • Respiratory virus season: Strategies for successful navigation
  • Matter of the heart: Prioritizing harm reduction in managing infective endocarditis associated with injection drug use
  • Effective but inaccessible antiobesity medications: A call for sharing responsibility for improving access to evidence-based care
Show more Commentary

Similar Articles

Subjects

  • Covid-19
  • Hospital Medicine
  • Infectious Diseases

Navigate

  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues
  • Supplements
  • Article Type
  • Specialty
  • CME/MOC Articles
  • CME/MOC Calendar
  • Media Kit

Authors & Reviewers

  • Manuscript Submission
  • Authors & Reviewers
  • Subscriptions
  • About CCJM
  • Contact Us
  • Cleveland Clinic Center for Continuing Education
  • Consult QD

Share your suggestions!

Copyright © 2025 The Cleveland Clinic Foundation. All rights reserved. The information provided is for educational purposes only. Use of this website is subject to the website terms of use and privacy policy. 

Powered by HighWire